This month’s topics:
Algebraic topology and the weather
The Lorenz attractor is probably the most famous chaotic dynamical system. It is the trajectory of the solution to a nonlinear system of three differential equations, discovered by Edward Lorenz (1963) in his attempts to model weather; it has come to epitomize the “butterfly effect.” (In this awesome animation you can watch the trajectory take shape). The trajectory of a three-dimensional chaotic dynamical system like the Lorenz system evolves in a region of 3-space that can be collapsed onto a 2-dimensional branched manifold (a smooth surface except along a 1-dimensional branching locus where it has Y-shaped cross-sections).

In an article in Chaos published October 12, 2021 (see the arXiv preprint), an international team (Gisela D. Charó, Mickaël D. Chekroun, Denisse Sciamarella, and Michael Ghil) examined random time-dependent perturbations of the Lorenz system. For each instant

The authors consider applications to Lorenz’s original problem, weather: “A fairly straightforward application [of this methodology] to the climate sciences might clarify […] the role of intermittent vs. oscillatory low-frequency variability in the atmosphere. […] Such phenomena include the so-called blocking of the westerlies and intraseasonal oscillations with periodicities of 40–50 days. They remark: “The framework introduced in this article to characterize such changes in topological features appears to hold promise for the understanding of topological tipping points in general.” (An online news service put two and two together and came up with: “Algebraic Topology Could be Used to Predict if and When Earth’s Climate System will Tip.”)
In conclusion: “We have concentrated throughout much of this paper on problems related to the climate sciences […]. With all due modesty, it is not unlikely — considering the great generality of topological methods — to expect the results obtained herein to have some applicability to other areas of the physical, life and socioeconomic sciences.”
Topology in children’s visual perception
It has been known at least since Jean Piaget’s 1948 work with Bärbel Inhelder, La représentation de l’espace chez l’enfant, that children start with basic topological distinctions (circle

Stimuli were presented in pairs, one stimulus on each side of the central fixation point. Participants, keeping their focus on the central point, had to record whether the two stimuli were the same or not by pressing one of the two specified keys on the keyboard. Reaction times were measured for each trial. In total, there were four types of combinations, that is, two eccentricities (central and peripheral) and two discrimination types (TP and non-TP).

The variable that the team measured, the normalized TP priority effect, was “computed from normalized reaction time differences between non-TP and TP trials,” so it measured how much a topological distinction speeded up the discrimination between two shapes.

As announced, the results show that whereas adults and children over 10 process TP differences faster than non-TP differences, both in their central and peripheral visual fields, this effect is almost completely absent in the peripheral vision of children aged 6-8.